Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Dev Sci ; 20(3)2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26613685

RESUMO

This study elucidates genetic influences on reflexive (as opposed to sustained) attention in children (aged 9-16 years; N = 332) who previously participated as infants in visual attention studies using orienting to a moving bar (Dannemiller, 2004). We investigated genetic associations with reflexive attention measures in infancy and childhood in the same group of children. The genetic markers (single nucleotide polymorphisms and variable number tandem repeats on the genes APOE, BDNF, CHRNA4, COMT, DRD4, HTR4, IGF2, MAOA, SLC5A7, SLC6A3, and SNAP25) are related to brain development and/or to the availability of neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine, dopamine, or serotonin. This study shows that typically developing children have differences in reflexive attention associated with their genes, as we found in adults (Lundwall, Guo & Dannemiller, 2012). This effort to extend our previous findings to outcomes in infancy and childhood was necessary because genetic influence may differ over the course of development. Although two of the genes that were tested in our adult study (Lundwall et al., 2012) were significant in either our infant study (SLC6A3) or child study (DRD4), the specific markers tested differed. Performance on the infant task was associated with SLC6A3. In addition, several genetic associations with an analogous child task occurred with markers on CHRNA4, COMT, and DRD4. Interestingly, the child version of the task involved an interaction such that which genotype group performed poorer on the child task depended on whether we were examining the higher or lower infant scoring group. These findings are discussed in terms of genetic influences on reflexive attention in infancy and childhood.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Reflexo/genética , Adolescente , Encéfalo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Proteínas da Membrana Plasmática de Transporte de Dopamina/genética , Marcadores Genéticos , Humanos , Repetições Minissatélites , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Receptores de Dopamina D4/genética
2.
BMC Neurosci ; 16: 66, 2015 Oct 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26471374

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Attention provides vital contribution to everyday functioning, and deficits in attention feature in many psychological disorders. Improved understanding of attention may eventually be critical to early identification and treatment of attentional deficits. One step in that direction is to acquire a better understanding of genetic associations with performance on a task measuring reflexive (exogenous) visual attention. Reflexive attention is an important component of overall attention because (along with voluntary selective attention) it participates in determining where attention is allocated and how susceptible to distractors the subject might be. The task that we used involves the presentation of a target that is preceded by one of several different types of cues (none, double, or single, either ipsilateral or contralateral to where the target subsequently appears). We used several different outcome measures depending on the cue presented. We have previously studied the relationship between selected genes and mean response time (RT). Here we report on the contributions of genetic markers to RT increases or decreases over the course of the task (linear trend in RT slope). RESULTS: Specifically, we find that RT slope for a variety of reflexive attention outcome measures is dependent on DAT1 genotype. DRD4 was near significant for one outcome measure in the final (best) model. APOE, COMT, and DBH were not significant in any models. CONCLUSIONS: It is especially interesting that genotype predicts linear changes in RT across trials (and not just mean differences or moment-to-moment variability). DAT1 is a gene that produces a protein involved in the transport of dopamine from the synapse. To our knowledge, this is the first study that has associated neurotransmitter genotypes with RT slope on a reflexive attention experiment. The direction of these effects is consistent with genetic risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). That is, those with two risk alleles for ADHD (6R/6R on the DAT1 intron 8 VNTR) either got slower as the task progressed or had the least improvement. Those with no risk alleles (5R/5R) had the most improvement in RT as the task progressed.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/genética , Atenção/fisiologia , Proteínas da Membrana Plasmática de Transporte de Dopamina/genética , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/genética , Adulto , Alelos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Predisposição Genética para Doença , Genótipo , Humanos , Modelos Genéticos
3.
PLoS One ; 7(2): e30731, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22348020

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Currently, there is a sense that the spatial orienting of attention is related to genotypic variations in cholinergic genes but not to variations in dopaminergic genes. However, reexamination of associations with both cholinergic and dopaminergic genes is warranted because previous studies used endogenous rather than exogenous cues and costs and benefits were not analyzed separately. Examining costs (increases in response time following an invalid pre-cue) and benefits (decreases in response time following a valid pre-cue) separately could be important if dopaminergic genes (implicated in disorders such as attention deficit disorder) independently influence the different processes of orienting (e.g., disengage, move, engage). METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We tested normal subjects (N = 161) between 18 and 61 years. Participants completed a computer task in which pre-cues preceded the presence of a target. Subjects responded (with a key press) to the location of the target (right versus left of fixation). The cues could be valid (i.e., appear where the target would appear) or invalid (appear contralateral to where the target would appear). DNA sequencing assays were performed on buccal cells to genotype known genetic markers and these were examined for association with task scores. Here we show significant associations between visual orienting and genetic markers (on COMT, DAT1, and APOE; R(2)s from 4% to 9%). CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: One measure in particular--the response time cost of a single dim, invalid cue - was associated with dopaminergic markers on COMT and DAT1. Additionally, variations of APOE genotypes based on the ε2/ε3/ε4 alleles were also associated with response time differences produced by simultaneous cues with unequal luminances. We conclude that individual differences in visual orienting are related to several dopaminergic markers as well as to a cholinergic marker. These results challenge the view that orienting is not associated with genotypic variation in dopaminergic genes.


Assuntos
Neurotransmissores/genética , Orientação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Acetilcolina/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Sinais (Psicologia) , Dopamina/genética , Marcadores Genéticos , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
4.
Infancy ; 12(1): 105-118, 2007 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33412729

RESUMO

This experiment investigated the impact of the path of approach of an object, from head on versus from the side, and the type of imminent contact with that object, a hit versus a miss, on young infants' perceptions of object looming. Consistent with earlier studies, we found that 4- to 5-month-old infants do indeed discriminate hits versus misses. We also found a novel result regarding the path of the approaching object. The discrimination of hits from misses was modified by whether or not the approaching objects passed in front of the infants' faces; objects crossing the line of sight evoked more frequent defensive reactions than objects that did not cross the line of sight, regardless of whether or not such objects were on a collision course. These findings are discussed within the context of the development of visually guided locomotion and linear versus nonlinear paths of translation through the world.

5.
J Vis ; 6(4): 366-78, 2006 Mar 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16889475

RESUMO

Previous research has shown that not all segments of a square frame are necessary to produce the illusory tilt of an enclosed vertical line. Indeed, the presence of a single tilted line is often sufficient to induce the illusory tilt of a nearby vertical line (Carpenter & Blakemore, 1973). How do the four segments of a quadrilateral frame contribute to the illusory tilt if any one of them is sufficient to induce the illusion? Response classification (RC) was used in two experiments to examine the independent contributions of the four segments of a quadrilateral frame to judgments of the direction of tilt of an enclosed vertical line. Orientation perturbations were added independently to the four frame segments. The orientation of the top segment contributed most systematically to these judgments, whereas the orientation of the bottom segment contributed very little. Individual differences were observed with two of the four observers showing the largest apparent tilt of the test line for shear configurations of the quadrilateral in which the top and bottom segments were rotated in a direction opposite to the right and left segments. Logistic regression was used with a double-pass technique to estimate the relative importance of the four segments. Interactions between the segments were not systematically related to the observers' judgments. The results are discussed in terms of the utility of RC and logistic regression for studying perceptual phenomena whose mechanisms are thought to lie at levels such as orientation that are different from those typically examined with RC and pixel noise.


Assuntos
Ilusões Ópticas , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Espacial , Percepção Visual , Classificação , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Julgamento , Modelos Logísticos
6.
Dev Sci ; 8(6): 567-82, 2005 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16246248

RESUMO

Very young infants orient overtly with eye and head movements to salient events in their visual environments, but those events rarely occur in the absence of competing visual stimuli. Two different models of how this kind of orienting is related to number and distribution of elements in the stimulus field were tested with infants across the age range from 2 to 5 months in four experiments. A set size manipulation in Experiments 1-3 produced data that were mostly inconsistent with the Maximum Response model proposed by Dannemiller (1998), especially at ages over 3 months. Experiment 4 produced data from 3.5-month-olds that were consistent with an alternative Dimensional Switching model that assumed that there was switching across trials in the stimulus dimension that drove orienting. This Dimensional Switching model can explain the small to nonexistent set size effects observed in the first three experiments as well as data from previous experiments using this paradigm. Factors that could produce this kind of dimensional switching over time were considered and other implications of this model for understanding the development of overt visual orienting were discussed.


Assuntos
Modelos Psicológicos , Orientação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Campos Visuais
7.
Spat Vis ; 17(3): 201-34, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15270546

RESUMO

Five experiments probed the conditions under which observers fail to report instantaneous reversals in the direction of motion of pixels that define the rotation of a transparent sphere or plane. Our results showed that the extent to which rotation reversals were not reported depended upon whether observers used strict or lax criteria to make their judgments, the degree of perspective present in the rotation simulations, and the percentage of pixels that actually reversed direction. Furthermore, we found failures to report rotation reversals both with stimuli whose pixels were confined to smooth surfaces and scattered within volumes. Reversal detection with planar stimuli, unlike sphere stimuli, depended upon the orientation of the stimulus at the moment of reversal. Treue et al. (1995) postulated a surface-interpolation process as the explanation for the apparent insensitivity of observers to such reversals. However, we suggest that other stages of processing (e.g. a structure-from-motion process) are required to account for these results.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Rotação , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Infancy ; 3(3): 275-301, 2002 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33451219

RESUMO

Young infants typically orient to a moving object, but the strength of this tendency depends on what else is in the visual field, with some objects competing for attention more effectively than others. This competition was studied in 3.5-month-old infants by manipulating the colors and spatial distributions of static elements that appeared with a small moving probe. The hypothesis was that the competition from these static bars would depend on their color contrasts. Three different color pairings were used: red with green, pink with green, and red with pink. The results were generally consistent with the hypothesis that the competition from static elements in the visual field depends on their color contrasts. Orienting at 3.5 months is determined by competition mechanisms that weight motion and color and most probably other stimulus characteristics to produce a directional response.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...